Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: https://ahro.austin.org.au/austinjspui/handle/1/18765
Full metadata record
DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorHarrington, Karra D-
dc.contributor.authorSchembri, Adrian-
dc.contributor.authorLim, Yen Ying-
dc.contributor.authorDang, Christa-
dc.contributor.authorAmes, David-
dc.contributor.authorHassenstab, Jason-
dc.contributor.authorLaws, Simon M-
dc.contributor.authorRainey-Smith, Stephanie R-
dc.contributor.authorRobertson, Joanne-
dc.contributor.authorRowe, Christopher C-
dc.contributor.authorSohrabi, Hamid R-
dc.contributor.authorSalvado, Olivier-
dc.contributor.authorWeinborn, Michael-
dc.contributor.authorVillemagne, Victor L-
dc.contributor.authorMasters, Colin L-
dc.contributor.authorMaruff, Paul-
dc.date2018-06-11-
dc.date.accessioned2018-08-31T06:05:34Z-
dc.date.available2018-08-31T06:05:34Z-
dc.date.issued2018-06-11-
dc.identifier.citationNeurobiology of aging 2018; 70: 170-179en
dc.identifier.urihttps://ahro.austin.org.au/austinjspui/handle/1/18765-
dc.description.abstractCognitive decline is considered an inevitable consequence of aging; however, estimates of cognitive aging may be influenced negatively by undetected preclinical Alzheimer's disease (AD). This study aimed to determine the extent to which estimates of cognitive aging were biased by preclinical AD. Cognitively normal older adults (n = 494) with amyloid-β status determined from positron emission tomography neuroimaging underwent serial neuropsychological assessment at 18-month intervals over 72 months. Estimates of the effects of age on verbal memory, working memory, executive function, and processing speed were derived using linear mixed models. The presence of preclinical AD and clinical progression to mild cognitive impairment or dementia during the study were then added to these models as covariates. Initially, age was associated with decline across all 4 cognitive domains. With the effects of elevated amyloid-β and clinical progression controlled, age was no longer associated with decline in verbal or working memory. However, the magnitude of decline was reduced only slightly for executive function and was unchanged for processing speed. Thus, considered together, the results of the study indicate that undetected preclinical AD negatively biases estimates of age-related cognitive decline for verbal and working memory.en
dc.language.isoeng-
dc.subjectAgingen
dc.subjectAlzheimeren
dc.subjectAmyloid-βen
dc.subjectCognitionen
dc.subjectPreclinicalen
dc.titleEstimates of age-related memory decline are inflated by unrecognized Alzheimer's disease.en
dc.typeJournal Articleen
dc.identifier.journaltitleNeurobiology of agingen
dc.identifier.affiliationThe Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australiaen
dc.identifier.affiliationCooperative Research Centre for Mental Health, Carlton, Victoria, Australiaen
dc.identifier.affiliationCogState Ltd., Melbourne, Victoria, Australiaen
dc.identifier.affiliationDepartment of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, Melbourne Medical School, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australiaen
dc.identifier.affiliationDepartment of Psychiatry, Academic Unit for Psychiatry of Old Age, The University of Melbourne, Parkville, Victoria, Australiaen
dc.identifier.affiliationNational Ageing Research Institute, Parkville, Victoria, Australiaen
dc.identifier.affiliationCharles F. and Joanne Knight Alzheimer's Disease Research Center, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USAen
dc.identifier.affiliationDepartment of Neurology, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USAen
dc.identifier.affiliationDepartment of Psychological & Brain Sciences, Washington University, St. Louis, MO, USAen
dc.identifier.affiliationCollaborative Genomics Group, Centre of Excellence for Alzheimer's Disease Research and Care, School of Exercise, Biomedical and Health Sciences, Edith Cowan University, Perth, Western Australia, Australiaen
dc.identifier.affiliationSchool of Biomedical Sciences, Faculty of Health Sciences, Curtin Health Innovation Research Institute, Curtin University, Western Australia, Australiaen
dc.identifier.affiliationCentre of Excellence for Alzheimer's Disease Research and Care, School of Exercise, Biomedical and Health Sciences, Edith Cowan University, Perth, Western Australia, Australiaen
dc.identifier.affiliationAustralian Alzheimer's Disease Research Unit, Hollywood Private Hospital, Perth, Western Australia, Australiaen
dc.identifier.affiliationDepartment of Molecular Imaging and Therapy, Austin Health, Heidelberg, Victoria, Australiaen
dc.identifier.affiliationDepartment of Medicine, Austin Health, The University of Melbourne, Heidelberg, Victoria, Australiaen
dc.identifier.affiliationSchool of Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, University of Western Australia, Nedlands, Western Australia, Australiaen
dc.identifier.affiliationCSIRO Health and Biosecurity, The Australian eHealth Research Centre, Brisbane, Queensland, Australiaen
dc.identifier.affiliationSchool of Psychological Science, University of Western Australia, Crawley, Western Australia, Australiaen
dc.identifier.doi10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.06.005en
dc.type.contentTexten
dc.identifier.orcid0000-0003-3910-2453en
dc.identifier.pubmedid30015036-
dc.type.austinJournal Article-
local.name.researcherMasters, Colin L
item.grantfulltextnone-
item.openairetypeJournal Article-
item.languageiso639-1en-
item.fulltextNo Fulltext-
item.openairecristypehttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_18cf-
item.cerifentitytypePublications-
crisitem.author.deptMolecular Imaging and Therapy-
crisitem.author.deptMolecular Imaging and Therapy-
crisitem.author.deptThe Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health-
Appears in Collections:Journal articles
Show simple item record

Page view(s)

38
checked on Nov 22, 2024

Google ScholarTM

Check


Items in AHRO are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved, unless otherwise indicated.